CSAIL Research Abstracts - 2005 link to http://publications.csail.mit.edu/abstracts/abstracts05/index.html link to http://www.csail.mit.edu
bullet Introduction bullet Architecture, Systems
& Networks
bullet Language, Learning,
Vision & Graphics
bullet Physical, Biological
& Social Systems
bullet Theory bullet

horizontal line

Perceptually-Motivated Image Processing

Sara L. Su, Frédo Durand & Maneesh Agrawala

Introduction

A major obstacle in photography is the presence of distracting elements that pull attention away from the main subject and clutter the composition. Photographers have developed post-processing techniques to reduce the salience of distractors by altering low-level features to which the visual system is particularly attuned: sharpness, brightness, chromaticity, or saturation. Biologically-inspired models of attention identify salient regions as statistical outliers in these feature distributions. One low-level feature that cannot be directly manipulated with existing image-editing software is texture variation. Psychophysical studies have shown that discontinuities in texture can elicit an edge perception similar to that triggered by color discontinuities.

Power Maps

First-order computational models of saliency measure the response to filter banks that extract contrast and orientation in the image. Various non-linearities can then be used to extract and combine maxima of the response to each feature. A recently-introduced second-order model performs additional image processing on the response to a first-order filter bank, effectively performing the same computation as first-order models but on what we term power maps rather than on image intensity. Higher-order features describing local frequency content, power maps have been used previously in image analysis; e.g. response to multiscale oriented filters can be used for texture discrimination. We show that power maps are also a powerful representation for manipulating frequency content in an image.

Texture Equalization

We introduce an image-processing technique for selectively reducing spatial variation of texture to reduce the salience of distracting regions. In a nutshell, our texture equalization technique modifies distracting regions to make them look more like uniform textures.

We illustrate our technique with a 1D example. The input signal (Fig. 1(a)) is first band-pass filtered (b) and rectified with an absolute value non-linearity (c). (For the 2D case, we use steerable pyramid filters to compute frequency content because they permit straightforward analysis, processing, and reconstruction of images.) Pooling the rectified response by applying a low-pass filter with a Gaussian kernel captures the local frequency content. We call the resulting image the power map (d).

To reduce texture variation in the image, some portion of the high frequencies of the power maps must be removed, a seemingly trivial image-processing operation. However, we must define how a modification of the power map translates into a modification of pyramid coefficients. The exponent of the high-pass response (e) is used to scale the bandpass response. Because the goal is to reduce variation, a negative multiple of the high-pass is used as the scale factor. Note how the scaled signal (f) has been `flattened' compared to the input. In the 2D case, the scaled subbands are then recombined to produce the final texture-equalized image.

Figure 1a Figure 1b Figure 1c Figure 1d Figure 1e Figure 1f
(a) Input (b) Band-pass (c) Rectified (d) Power map (e) Scale (f) Output
Figure 1: Texture discrimination and manipulation in 1D.
Psychophysical Study

To validate our technique's effectiveness, qualitative changes in user fixations on original and modified images were recorded using an eye tracker. Emphasized regions attracted and held fixations longer than de-emphasized ones. Results of a search experiment quantified the effect of our technique on response time. Subjects were asked to find a target object in a series of images, some unmodified and some in which distractors had been de-emphasized. Texture equalization resulted in a search speedup of more than 20%.

Discussion

Texture equalization is complementary to de-emphasis methods such as Gaussian blur, which increases depth-of-field effects. Reduced sharpness can be undesirable, particularly if distractors are at the same distance as the main subject. Blur removes high frequencies and emphasizes medium ones, possibly resulting in a more distracting object, while our technique makes high-frequencies more uniform, "camouflaging" medium-frequency content. Our technique is most effective for textured image regions, while Gaussian blur works best when small depth-of-field effects are already present and when medium-frequency content is not distracting.

Acknowledgements

This material is based on work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. 0429739 and the Graduate Research Fellowship Program, MIT Project Oxygen, and the Royal Dutch/Shell Group.

References:

[1] Sara L. Su, Frédo Durand, and Maneesh Agrawala. De-Emphasis of Distracting Image Regions Using Texture Power Maps. Submitted to ICCV 2005.

[2] Sara L. Su, Frédo Durand, and Maneesh Agrawala. De-Emphasis of Distracting Image Regions Using Texture Power Maps. MIT Laboratory for Computer Science Technical Report MIT-LCS-TR-987, April 2005.

Figure 2a
(a) Input

Figure 2b (b) Gaussian blurring of background

Figure 2c (c) Texture equalization of background

Figure 2: Specular highlights in the leaves and other distractors prevent clear foreground / background separation in the original photograph. Gaussian blur de-emphasizes the masked region by introducing depth-of-field effects. The reduced sharpness is undesirable because of the new conflicting depth cues between the two tigers, which should appear at the same distance. Reducing texture variation in the background effectively de-emphasizes without this effect.
horizontal line

MIT logo Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory (CSAIL)
The Stata Center, Building 32 - 32 Vassar Street - Cambridge, MA 02139 - USA
tel:+1-617-253-0073 - publications@csail.mit.edu
(Note: On July 1, 2003, the AI Lab and LCS merged to form CSAIL.)